IV Estimators: Are We Really Measuring What We Think We Are?
"A Fresh Look at Instrumental Variable Estimators: Understanding When Linear Models Fall Short and What to Do About It"
In the world of economics and social sciences, researchers often grapple with the challenge of isolating causal relationships. One powerful tool in their arsenal is the instrumental variable (IV) estimator, designed to disentangle the effects of a specific variable from other confounding factors. The foundational idea is brilliantly simple: leverage an external factor—the instrumental variable—that influences the variable of interest but doesn't directly affect the outcome, except through the channel we're studying. This instrument acts like a surgical scalpel, allowing us to pinpoint the true causal effect.
However, like any sophisticated tool, IV estimators come with their own set of assumptions and potential pitfalls. One critical assumption, often glossed over, is monotonicity. Monotonicity, in its simplest form, suggests that the instrument should consistently affect the treatment variable in the same direction for everyone in the population. But what happens when this assumption breaks down? What if the instrument encourages treatment for some individuals while discouraging it for others?
New research is questioning the validity of commonly used techniques. It suggests that a seemingly straightforward application of linear IV estimators might lead us astray, particularly when the monotonicity assumption is compromised. But do not panic! This article will walk you through these potential issues, explain the nuances of 'weak monotonicity,' and introduce alternative approaches that can help you navigate these treacherous waters, ensuring that your causal inferences remain robust and reliable.
When Linearity Meets Reality: The Problem with Standard IV Estimators

Imagine you are studying the effect of education on income, and you use the availability of colleges in a student's hometown as an instrument. The monotonicity assumption would imply that having more colleges nearby either encourages or has no effect on everyone's educational attainment; it cannot discourage anyone. But what if, for some students, the presence of many colleges actually reduces their likelihood of pursuing higher education because they feel overwhelmed by choices or decide to enter the workforce directly? This is a case where the monotonicity assumption fails.
- Negative Weights: Standard IV estimators can assign negative weights to some groups when monotonicity is violated, distorting the average treatment effect.
- Misinterpretation: This distortion can lead to completely wrong conclusions about the causal relationship.
- Weak Monotonicity: When the instrument affects sub-groups in different directions, the reliability of the IV estimator drops dramatically.
The Path Forward: Embracing Complexity for Better Insights
The world is complex, and human behavior is rarely uniform. By embracing more nuanced methodologies like interacted specifications and carefully testing our assumptions, we can unlock richer, more reliable insights into the causal forces that shape our world. Don't let violations of monotonicity undermine your hard work. With the right tools and a healthy dose of skepticism, you can navigate the challenges of causal inference and make truly impactful discoveries.