Is Your Working Memory Sabotaging Your Counting Skills? What the Science Says
"Discover how phonological working memory impacts your ability to count, and why subitizing might be your secret weapon for quick assessments."
Quickly assessing the number of items in front of us seems like a simple task, but it actually relies on complex cognitive processes. When we look at a small group of objects, we can instantly recognize the quantity – this is called "subitizing." But when the number of items increases, we switch to a more effortful process: counting. Recent research sheds light on the distinct mental mechanisms behind these two processes and how our working memory affects them.
For years, experts have debated whether subitizing and counting rely on the same or different cognitive mechanisms. Some theories propose a single, unified process, while others suggest that subitizing leverages visual skills, while counting depends more on working memory. The latest research explores the specific role of different components of working memory – particularly phonological and spatial working memory – in these enumeration processes.
This article unpacks a study by Sarit Ashkenazi that examines how phonological and spatial working memory impact our ability to subitize and count. By understanding these mechanisms, we can gain insights into how our minds process numerical information and potentially improve our cognitive performance.
Counting vs. Subitizing: Unpacking the Cognitive Differences

Enumeration, the act of determining the quantity of items in a set, relies on two primary processes: subitizing and serial counting. Subitizing allows for the rapid and accurate recognition of quantities up to about three or four items. Serial counting, on the other hand, is a slower, more deliberate process used for larger quantities.
- Individualizing and localizing each object.
- Switching spatial attention from one object to the next.
- Summing the number of objects.
- Inhibiting already counted objects.
Key Takeaways: Working Memory's Impact on Enumeration
Ashkenazi's study provides compelling evidence for the distinct roles of working memory in subitizing and serial counting. The key finding is that phonological working memory, which involves the temporary storage and manipulation of verbal information, significantly affects serial counting. When participants were under phonological load (i.e., retaining non-words in memory), their performance in counting decreased. However, phonological load did not affect subitizing abilities.
Interestingly, spatial working memory, which involves the temporary storage and manipulation of spatial information, did not significantly impact either counting or subitizing. This suggests that while spatial attention is involved in both processes, it doesn't rely heavily on spatial working memory resources.
These findings support the canonical enumeration theory, which posits that subitizing and serial counting are supported by distinct cognitive mechanisms. Serial counting relies heavily on phonological working memory, while subitizing relies more on visual-spatial object individuation capacity. Understanding these cognitive differences can help us develop strategies to improve our enumeration skills and potentially address difficulties in mathematical cognition.